You might be underestimating strangers’ interest in your life

"We wrongly assume that other people are somewhat indifferent towards us, so we avoid more intimate conversation, thinking it would be awkward," says Amit Kumar. "But we'd likely be happier if we dug deeper when we're interacting with others."(Credit: Getty Images)

People benefit from deep conversations, but we often stick to small talk with strangers because we underestimate how much they’re interested in our lives, according to new research.

The findings have important practical implications, especially as the pandemic wanes and people become more social again.

“People care about what we have to say, just as we care about what they have to say.”

“We wrongly assume that other people are somewhat indifferent towards us, so we avoid more intimate conversation, thinking it would be awkward,” says coauthor Amit Kumar, an assistant professor of marketing at the University of Texas at Austin’s McCombs School of Business. “But we’d likely be happier if we dug deeper when we’re interacting with others.”

For the study, which appears in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, researchers designed a series of 12 experiments with more than 1,800 total participants. The researchers asked pairs of people—mainly strangers—to discuss either relatively deep or shallow topics.

In some experiments, people received shallow or deep questions to discuss. Shallow questions included typical small-talk topics, such as, “What is the best TV show you’ve seen in the last month? Tell your partner about it,” or “What do you think about the weather today?”

Deep questions elicited more personal and intimate information, such as, “Can you describe a time you cried in front of another person?” or “If a crystal ball could tell you the truth about yourself, your life, your future, or anything else, what would you want to know?”

In other experiments, people generated their own deep and shallow conversation topics.

Before the conversations, participants predicted how awkward they thought the conversations would be, how connected they thought they would feel to their conversation partner, and how much they would enjoy the conversation. Afterward, they rated the awkwardness, connectedness, and enjoyment they actually felt.

Overall, the researchers found that both deep and shallow conversations felt less awkward and led to greater feelings of connectedness and enjoyment than the participants had expected. That effect tended to be stronger for deep conversations.

In one experiment, participants who had a deep conversation with one partner and a shallow conversation with another partner initially expected to prefer the shallow conversation but actually preferred the deep one.

If deep conversations are genuinely better and people in these experiments says they wanted to have deep conversations, then why aren’t they having more of them?

The researchers suspect it might be because people underestimate how interested strangers are in learning about their deeper thoughts and feelings. In some of the experiments, the researchers asked participants to predict how interested their conversation partner would be in the discussion, and then afterward to indicate how interested their partner actually was in the discussion.

On average, people consistently underestimated how interested their partners would be in learning about them.

“People care about what we have to say, just as we care about what they have to say,” Kumar says. “And these effects are quite robust. Our different experiments recruited students from around the world, online participants, and people who just happened to be at a public park.

“We even had older business executives at a financial services firm talking about the last time they cried in front of another person. Across all these different samples of participants, we find similar effects.”

In the final experiments, the researchers examined whether having more accurate expectations about a conversation partner increased people’s interest in having a deeper conversation. In one experiment, they told the participants to imagine that they would be speaking to a particularly caring and interested person, or to a particularly uncaring and uninterested one. Participants who expected they would be speaking to the caring person chose to discuss deeper questions.

In another experiment, the researchers told people about the results of the previous experiments—letting them know that most people underestimate the degree to which other people are interested in hearing about their personal and deeper thoughts. People given this information later chose to discuss deeper questions with strangers more often than people not given the information.

“In these social situations, the fear is largely in our heads—how we imagine these conversations going,” Kumar says. “Reality suggests we might be better off if we opened up and dove a little deeper than we normally do.”

Additional coauthors are from Northwestern University and the University of Chicago.

Source: UT Austin