Preschoolers learn words best if they have a nap

(Credit: Getty Images)

Naps play an important role in helping babies hang onto what they learn. Now, a new study suggests naptime could have a similar effect on language learning in preschoolers.

Researchers studied verb learning in three-year-olds and found that those who napped after learning new verbs had a better understanding of the words when tested 24 hours later.

The findings, which will appear in the journal Child Development, suggest that even though preschool is a time when naps start to dwindle, parents might want to hang onto the practice for a bit longer.

‘Blicking’ and ‘rooping’

Researchers tested 39 typically developing three-year-olds, divided into two groups: habitual nappers (those who nap four or more days a week) and non-habitual nappers (those who nap three or fewer days per week). Within each group, children were randomly assigned to either a napping condition, in which they would nap for at least 30 minutes after learning a new verb, or a wakefulness condition, in which they would not nap after learning.

The children were taught two made-up verbs—”blicking” and “rooping”—and were shown a video in which two different actors performed separate whole-body actions to correspond with each verb.

Twenty-four hours later, the children were shown videos of two new actors performing the same actions they learned the previous day and were asked to point at which person was “blicking” and which was “rooping.”

Why a catnap at work is a nifty idea

Children who had napped within about an hour of learning the verbs performed better than those who stayed awake for at least five hours after learning, regardless of whether they were habitual nappers.

Different actors were used in the training and testing videos to allow researchers to measure how well children “generalized” the new verbs, meaning they were able to recognize them even when performed in a different context by different people.

Verbs aren’t ‘neatly packaged’

“We’re interested in generalization because that’s the target for word learning. You have to be able to generalize words to be able to use them productively in language,” says Michelle Sandoval, a graduate student in psychology at the University of Arizona.

“Regardless of typical napping behavior, children who were in the sleep condition—who were asked to nap after learning—were the ones who generalized, and those who stayed awake were not able to generalize 24 hours later.”

Researchers chose to study verb learning since verbs typically are more difficult to learn than simple nouns that often are children’s first words, such as “mommy,” “daddy,” and “doggie.”

“Verbs are interesting because we know they are very challenging for children to learn and to retain over time,” Sandoval says. “Individual objects have clear boundaries, and children learn about those very early in development—before they hit their first birthday, they know a lot about objects.

“Verbs aren’t as neatly packaged. Besides a physically perceptible action, a verb contains information about the number of people involved and can contain information about when the action took place.”

Slow-wave sleep

Researchers were interested in napping’s effects on preschoolers in particular because that tends to be an age when children start napping less. While an infant between birth and six months old may take up to six naps a day, many children are down to one nap or no naps a day by preschool.

The learning benefit of napping could come from what is known as slow-wave sleep, researchers say.

Language at 3 predicts 3rd grade depression risk

“There’s a lot of evidence that different phases of sleep contribute to memory consolidation, and one of the really important phases is slow-wave sleep, which is one of the deepest forms of sleep,” says coauthor Rebecca Gómez, associate professor of psychology, cognitive science, and second language acquisition and teaching.

“What’s really important about this phase is that essentially what the brain is doing is replaying memories during sleep, so those brain rhythms that occur during slow-wave sleep and other phases of non-REM sleep are actually reactivating those patterns—those memories—and replaying them and strengthening them.

While it appears that napping could continue to benefit 3-year-olds’ learning, parents shouldn’t necessarily fret if they can’t get their preschooler to nap during the day, since there is a lot of variability in children’s sleep behaviors at that age, Gómez says.

The most important thing is total amount of sleep. Preschool-age children should be getting 10 to 12 hours of sleep in a 24-hour period, whether it’s all at night or a combination of nighttime sleep and napping.

“We know that when children don’t get enough sleep it can have long-term consequences,” including deficits on cognitive tests, Gómez says.

“It’s important to create opportunities for children to nap—to have a regular time in their schedule that they could do that.”

Source: University of Arizona