Babies rely on the observations of others

"We think that the altercentric bias facilitates the child's learning at a unique time in life when motoric immaturity limits the infant's interaction with the environment," Velisar Manea suggests. (Credit: Getty Images)

Eight-month-old babies rely more on other people’s attention than on their own observations, a study shows.

Researchers at the University of Copenhagen studied the ability of eight- and twelve-month-old infants to remember the location of a moving object. The aim of the project is to test a theory that early in infancy there is a so-called altercentric bias: The infant trusts other’s observations more than their own.

First, the researchers investigated whether infants as young as eight months can remember the location of an object if it is moved from one hidden location to another. To do this, the researchers used an animation showing a conveyor belt or a hand moving a ball behind one screen and then behind another screen.

“When we then reveal either location as empty, the children look longer at the place where the ball should be. This shows us that the children have a memory of where the object moved to,” says project leader Velisar Manea, a postdoctoral researcher in the psychology department.

In the initial trials, eight-month-old children had to follow a ball being moved by either a conveyor belt or a hand. In both cases, the children were able to figure out where the ball should be, even if it was mysteriously missing.

To investigate how the attention of others affects infants’ memory, the researchers then conducted an experiment in which an animated human character also followed the movement of the ball.

“While the ball is being transported to its first location, the animated character looks at the ball. Then we cover the character and the infants are left alone to watch the movement of the ball to the second location,” explains Manea. “As predicted, babies expected to see the ball in the first location, even though they had seen it being moved to the second location. They prioritized the animated agent’s attention to what they saw afterwards.”

The research team conducted a control experiment where the animated agent follows the ball’s location from start to finish.

“To our surprise, infants looked equally to both revealed locations in this experiment. Once again, the eight-month-olds possibly expected the ball on both locations, as the agent attended both,” says Manea.

In the next trials, the infants went through a similar experiment, but the movement of the ball was fully or partially followed by an animated human. Here, the children’s attention was dependent on the character’s gaze when judging where the ball was.

But when does the child start to trust their own observations? The research team investigated this question by conducting similar experiments with 12-month-old children.

“Unlike the eight-month-old children, the 12-month-olds were able to remember the last position of the ball in the experiment, where the agent also saw the final location,” says Manea.

In contrast, when the agent only saw the ball in the first hiding location, but the babies saw the transfer to the final location alone, they looked equally to both places.

“This suggests that 12-month-old children are in a transitional phase, where some infants are less affected by the perspective of others, while others are still strongly influenced,” says Manea.

So why is the infant’s memory built to initially rely more on the observations of others—and then later become more independent?

“We think that the altercentric bias facilitates the child’s learning at a unique time in life when motoric immaturity limits the infant’s interaction with the environment,” Manea suggests.

The findings appear in the Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

Collaborators on the project are from the University of Copenhagen, the Max Plank Institute in Leipzig, and the Central European University in Vienna.

Source: University of Copenhagen