children's health

Nurturing moms negate prenatal stress


“This is such refreshing news for mothers,” says study author Thomas O’Connor. “Pregnancy is an emotional experience for many women, and there is already so much for mothers to be careful of and concerned about. It’s a relief to learn that, by being good parents, they might ‘buffer’ their babies against potential setbacks.” (Courtesy: iStockphoto)

U. ROCHESTER (US)—A loving bond between mother and child early in life can help protect children from the damaging effects of prenatal exposure to stress hormone—known to be a harbinger for poor cognitive development.

Published in the journal Biological Psychiatry, the study represents the first direct human evidence that fetuses exposed to elevated levels of the stress hormone cortisol may have trouble paying attention or solving problems later on. But what may be more intriguing is the study’s second finding—that this negative link disappears almost entirely if the mother forges a secure connection with her baby.

“Our results shape the argument that fetal exposure to cortisol—which may in part be controlled by the mother’s stress level—and early care giving experience combine to influence a child’s neurodevelopment,” says study author Thomas O’Connor, professor of psychiatry and of psychology at the University of Rochester Medical Center.

“If future studies confirm these findings, we’ll need to not only engineer ways to reduce stress in pregnancy, but we’ll need to also promote sensitive care giving by moms and dads.”

For the study, researchers recruited 125 women at an amniocentesis clinic in an urban maternity hospital, taking a sample of their amniotic fluid so that stress hormones in it could be measured.

The mothers were on average at 17 weeks gestation. Only mothers with normal, healthy pregnancies and subsequent deliveries were followed.

When their children reached 17 months of age, researchers administered a Bailey infant developmental scale test, which relies on puzzles, pretend play, and baby “memory” challenges to gauge youngsters’ cognitive development.

They also observed the baby and mother using the Ainsworth “Strange Situation” test, which judge’s childrearing quality, categorizing mom-baby pairs as either showing secure or insecure attachment to each other.

With cortisol levels, relationship quality results, and cognition scores in hand, researchers analyzed how the first two measures might influence the third. Researchers found that with children showing “insecure attachment” to their mothers, a high prenatal cortisol level was linked with shorter attention spans and weaker language and problem-solving skills.

But interestingly, for toddlers who enjoyed secure relationships with their moms, any negative link between high prenatal cortisol exposure and kids’ cognitive development was eliminated.

“This is such refreshing news for mothers,” O’Connor says. “Pregnancy is an emotional experience for many women, and there is already so much for mothers to be careful of and concerned about. It’s a relief to learn that, by being good parents, they might ‘buffer’ their babies against potential setbacks.”

O’Connor goes on to note a couple important nuances of the study.

The first is that the amniotic (in-uteri) cortisol studied could result from two sources, and it’s hard to pinpoint which. It might, for instance, be passed along the placenta from an anxious mother to her unborn baby—or it could be created and excreted directly by a stressed fetus itself.

“While many large-scale studies have observed that prenatal stress may influence child development, our particular study sheds some light on the ‘how’,” O’Connor explains.

“Still, much more research is needed to better pinpoint the exact mechanisms behind a mother ‘transferring’ her stress to her unborn baby.”

This study plays into the much larger theory of “fetal programming,” which suggests that events in the womb may prime the developing child for long-term health and developmental outcomes.

Past studies, for instance, have found a pregnant mother’s diet can sway a child’s long-term risk for heart disease, diabetes and obesity. Along with diet, prenatal stress has emerged as another large-looming factor in such programming.

“Our results support this emerging theory,” says London-based study coauthor Valetta Glover. “In neurology, the idea emerging is that unborn children sense their mothers’ stress hormone levels, programming them for greater watchfulness. We’re trying to determine whether or not that sensitivity comes with greater anxiety during childhood, and if so, what we can do about it.”

The researchers plan to revisit these same children when they turn 6; when they hope to give the group a battery of more definitive tests to see how the interplay between in-uteri cortical levels and sensitive parenting pans out in the long-term.

Those tests would include imaging studies of the children’s brains, looking to see if the higher cortical levels may be linked to anatomical changes.

Researchers from the Institute of Reproductive and Developmental Biology at Imperial College in London contributed to the study, which was supported by grants from the March of Dimes and the National Institute of Mental Health, part of the National Institutes of Health.

University of Rochester health news:

Related Articles