View more articles about

cognition

MRI predicts video gaming skills

U ILLINOIS (US) — It’s possible to predict how well you’ll perform a complex task—like playing a difficult video game—by looking closely at one area of your brain: the basal ganglia.

“There are many, many studies, hundreds perhaps, in which psychometricians, people who do the quantitative analysis of learning, try to predict from SATs, GREs, MCATs, or other tests how well you’re going to succeed at something,” says University of Illinois psychology professor Art Kramer, who led the research.

These methods, along with studies that look at the relative size of specific-brain structures, have had some success predicting learning, Kramer says, “but never to this degree in a task that is so complex.”

Using magnetic resonance imaging and a method known as multivoxel pattern analysis, the researchers found significant differences in patterns of a particular type of MRI signal, called T2*, in the basal ganglia of study subjects. This group of brain structures is known to be important for procedural learning, coordinated movement and feelings of reward.

Researchers were able to predict between 55 and 68 percent of the variance (differences in performance) among the 34 people who later learned to play the game by analyzing the differences in patterns. Full details of the study are reported in the journal PLoS ONE.

“We take a fresh look at MRI images that are recorded routinely to investigate brain function,” says Ohio State University psychology professor Dirk Bernhardt-Walther, who helped design and perform the computational analysis. “By analyzing these images in a new way, we find variations among participants in the patterns of brain activity in their basal ganglia.”

He says powerful statistical algorithms allowed the researchers to connect the patterns to learning success. “Our method may be useful for predicting differences in abilities of individuals in other contexts as well,” he continues. “Testing this would be inexpensive because the method recycles MRI images that are recorded in many studies anyway.”

After having their brains imaged, participants spent 20 hours learning to play Space Fortress, a video game developed at the University of Illinois in which players try to destroy a fortress without losing their own ship to one of several potential hazards. None of the subjects had much experience with video games prior to the study.

The game, which was designed to test participants’ real-world cognitive skills, is quite challenging, Kramer notes. It forces players to frequently shift their attention to pursue various goals or avoid threats.

When they are first learning to play, study subjects “tend to start out with negative 2,000 points,” he says. After 20 hours of training and practice, all the players’ scores go up significantly. Some do much better than others, however, a difference that can be predicted to a large degree by analyzing activity in parts of the basal ganglia.

“We predict up to three times as much of the variance (in learning) as you would using performance measures,” Kramer says. The researchers tested their results against other measures and replicated the findings in new trials with different study subjects.

The brain regions the researchers analyzed include the caudate nucleus and the putamen. These brain structures are active when one is engaged in learning new motor skills, such as moving a joystick, but they also are important in tasks that require one to strategize and quickly shift one’s attention.

A third region, the nucleus accumbens is known to process emotions associated with reward or punishment.

The researchers found that patterns of activity in the putamen and caudate nucleus were better predictors of future performance than those in the nucleus accumbens. They also found that analyzing white matter (the axons and dendrites that carry signals between neurons), but not gray matter (the cell bodies), offered the best predictive power.

“Our data suggest that some persistent physiological and or neuroanatomical difference is actually the predictor of learning,” Kramer says.

The findings should not be interpreted to mean that some people are destined to succeed or fail at a given task or learning challenge, however, Kramer notes. “We know that many of these components of brain structure and function are changeable.”

More news from the University of Illinois: www.beckman.illinois.edu

Related Articles