Consumers can’t taste what wine buffs sense

PENN STATE (US) — Wine experts’ acute sense of taste may make their recommendations irrelevant to consumers not born with the same ability.

“What we found is that the fundamental taste ability of an expert is different,” says John Hayes, assistant professor of food science and director of Penn State‘s sensory evaluation center. “And, if an expert’s ability to taste is different from the rest of us, should we be listening to their recommendations?”

In a taste test, wine experts showed more sensitivity to tastes than average wine consumers.

[sources]

Hayes says that the participants sampled an odorless chemical—propylthiouracil—that is used to measure a person’s reaction to bitter tastes.  People with acute tasting ability will find the chemical—also referred to as PROB, or probe—extremely bitter, while people with normal tasting abilities say it has a slightly bitter taste, or is tasteless.

The researchers, who report their findings in the current issue of the American Journal of Enology and Viticulture, say that wine experts were significantly more likely to find the chemical more bitter than non-experts.

“Just like people can be color blind, they can also be taste blind,” says Hayes.

Hayes, who worked with Gary Pickering, professor of biological sciences and psychology/wine science at Brock University in Ontario, Canada, says that the acute taste of wine experts may mean that expert recommendations in wine magazines and journals may be too subtle for average wine drinkers to sense.

The researchers also found that people who were more adventurous in trying new foods were also more willing to drink new types of wines and alcoholic beverages, but this food adventurousness did not necessarily predict wine expertise. While wine experts were more likely to try new wines and alcoholic beverages, Hayes says they were not more likely to try new foods.

Wine critics typically rate wines on a 100-point quality scale that incorporates a range of characteristics, including tartness, sweetness and fruitiness, varietal typicity, and over all liking, among others. Their descriptions of the wines can be specific, highlighting grapefruit or grassy notes, or the balance of sugar and acid.

However, according to Hayes, average wine consumers probably cannot discern these subtle differences between wines. While prior experience matters, biology seems to play a role.

Prior to the taste test, the researchers passed out short questionnaires to determine the involvement in the wine industry of 330 participants at wine-tasting events in Ontario. Based on the answers to the questionnaire, they divided the people into two groups: wine consumers and wine experts.

Approximately 110 of the participants indicated that they were professional winemakers, wine writers, liquor control agents, and wine judges and were classified as wine experts. The researchers classified all the other participants as non-experts.

“Statistically, the two groups were very different in how they tasted our bitter probe compound,” says Hayes.

Hayes says that previous studies have shown that biological factors may explain the acute taste of experts. Many wine experts may be drawn to careers in the wine industry based on their enhanced ability to taste. While learning plays a role in their expertise and other factors matter, such as how they communicate their thoughts and opinions on wines, some wine experts may have an innate advantage in learning to discern small differences in wine.

“It’s not just learning,” says Hayes. “Experts also appear to differ at a biological level.”

More news from Penn State: http://live.psu.edu/