Can messages about hard work make us strive?

The study investigates common messages about the nature of abilities, such as a teacher comforting a student ("It's OK, not everyone can be a math person") or a sports announcer commenting on a player's skill ("Wow, what a natural!"). (Credit: "fortune cookie" via Shutterstock)

The nature versus nurture debate continues, but new research shows that just telling people that hard work trumps genes causes instant changes in the brain.

That message about hard work may make them more willing to strive for success, according to the new study.

The findings suggest the human brain is more receptive to the message that intelligence comes from the environment, regardless of whether it’s true. And this simple message, says lead investigator Hans Schroder, may ultimately prompt us to work harder.

“Giving people messages that encourage learning and motivation may promote more efficient performance,” says Schroder, a doctoral student in clinical psychology at Michigan State University.

“In contrast, telling people that intelligence is genetically fixed may inadvertently hamper learning.”

Hans Schroder puts cap on brain study participant
In his research, Hans Schroder, standing, measures people’s brain activity to investigate how they respond to mistakes. (Credit: G.L. Kohuth/Michigan State)

Smart or hard working?

[related]

In past research by Stanford University psychologist Carol Dweck, elementary students performing a task were either praised for their intelligence (“You’re so smart!”) or for their effort (“You worked really hard!”) after correct responses.

As the task became harder, children in the first group performed worse after their mistakes compared to the group that had heard effort was important.

The new study, which appears online in the journal Biological Psychology, offers what could be the first physiological evidence to support those findings, in the form of a positive brain response.

“These subtle messages seem to have a big impact, and now we can see they have an immediate impact on how the brain handles information about performance,” Schroder says.

Testing brain response

For the study, two groups of participants read different articles. One article reported that intelligence is largely genetic, while the other said the brilliance of da Vinci and Einstein was “probably due to a challenging environment. Their genius had little to do with genetic structure.”

Participants were instructed to remember the main points of the article and completed a simple computer task while their brain activity was recorded. The findings, in a nutshell:

The group that read intelligence was primarily genetic paid more attention to their responses, as if they were more concerned with their performance. This extra attention, however, did not relate to performance on trials after errors, suggesting a disconnection between brain and behavior.

In contrast, those who had read that intelligence was due to a challenging environment showed a more efficient brain response after they made a mistake, possibly because they believed they could do better on the next trial. The more attention these participants paid to mistakes, the faster their responses were on the next trial.

Nature vs. nurture

The study does not weigh in on the age-old “nature vs. nurture” debate, Schroder notes. Rather, it investigates the messages about the nature of abilities people are exposed to on a regular basis, from a teacher comforting a student (“It’s OK, not everyone can be a math person”) to the sports announcer commenting on a player’s skill (“Wow, what a natural!”).

These messages are thought to contribute to the attitudes or “mindsets” people hold about their intelligence and abilities.

The research started as part of Schroder’s honors thesis as an undergraduate working in the Clinical Psychophysiology Lab directed by Jason Moser, assistant professor at Michigan State.

Source: Michigan State University