looking_through_porthole_525

To get creative, shed brain’s thought filter

U. PENNSYLVANIA (US) — Blocking the brain’s prefrontal cortex—the part that works as a kind of filter to keep out irrelevant thoughts—can improve performance when completing creative tasks, new research shows.

Previous studies have shown that the prefrontal cortex—in particular, the left prefrontal cortex—is one important area of the brain that supports cognitive control.

As a test of whether reduced cognitive control might be advantageous in some circumstances, researchers designed an experiment that involved inhibiting the activity of the left prefrontal cortex in adults while they completed a creative task.

In the task, participants were shown pictures of everyday objects and were asked to quickly come up with uses for them that are out of the ordinary, such as using a baseball bat as a rolling pin. Participants saw a sequence of 60 objects, one every nine seconds, and the researchers measured how long it took for them to come up with a valid response, or if they were unable to do so before the next picture appears.


Researchers place the electrodes of this tDCS system so that the current passes through the area of the brain being studied. (Credit: U. Pennsylvania)

In the study, published in Cognitive Neuroscience, the researchers hypothesized that high levels of cognitive control would be a detriment to coming up with these kinds of uncommon uses.

“When we use objects in daily life, our cognitive control helps us focus on what the object is typically used for and ‘filters out’ irrelevant properties,” says Evangelia Chrysikou, assistant professor at the University of Kansas who worked in the lab of Sharon Thompson-Schill, professor of psychology at the University of Pennsylvania at the time of the research.

“However, to come up with the idea of using a baseball bat as a rolling pin, you have to consider things like its shape and the material it’s made of.”

“The real takeaway,” says Thompson-Schill, “is that when you give people a task for which they do not know the goal—such as showing them an object and asking, ‘What else can you do with this thing’—anything that they would normally do to filter out irrelevant information about the object will hurt their ability to do the task.”

Experiments to test such hypotheses have been aided by new ways of non-invasively manipulating neurons in specific areas of the brain, inducing a variety of temporary changes in perception and performance.

The method the team used, called transcranial direct current stimulation, or tDCS, involves passing a weak electrical charge through the brain, aiming the charge’s path so it intersects with areas thought to be associated with an ability or behavior. This charge can influence the electrical activity that constitutes cell-to-cell communication in those areas.

“TDCS is believed to induce incremental shifts in the electrical potential of neuronal membranes, making it more or less likely that neurons will reach their threshold for firing,” says Roy Hamilton. “In this instance, we employed stimulation in a way that would make it harder for neurons to fire, thereby diminishing behaviorally relevant activity in that part of the brain.”

Participants were first split into groups corresponding to three experimental conditions: one would receive tDCS to their left prefrontal cortex for the duration of the task, another would receive it to their right prefrontal cortex and a third would receive what amounted to a placebo.

TDCS produces a slight tingling sensation on the scalp when it is first applied, so those in the third group received only a brief period of stimulation before the task began, rather than throughout.

As additional controls, each of these three groups was also split in half, with one set completing the uncommon-use task and the other simply stating what the object is normally used for. And all participants also completed a task that involved remembering strings of numbers, a common exercise in psychological experiments that has been shown not to require the prefrontal cortex.

A huge difference

“We wouldn’t want to think that the stimulation affected everything,” Thompson-Schill says. “So if we found an effect when participants were remembering numbers, we’d be worried about our interpretation of the data.”

As expected, none of the experimental conditions affected participants’ performance when asked to recall the sequences of numbers, or when they were asked to say the common uses of the objects they saw.

But there was a marked difference between those who received tDCS to their left prefrontal cortex and those who didn’t when completing the uncommon-use task.

The right prefrontal cortex and placebo groups couldn’t come up with uncommon uses for an average of 15 out of 60 objects, whereas those whose left prefrontal cortices were being inhibited only missed an average of eight.  The latter group was also able to provide correct responses an average of a second faster than the former two.

“A second faster difference is huge in psychology research. We’re used to seeing differences measured in milliseconds,” Thompson-Schill says. “This is probably the biggest effect I’ve seen over my 20 years in research.”

These results lend credence to the idea that high levels of cognitive control may be a disadvantage in some circumstances, such as in early development.

“We differ from non-human primates in having a long period of immaturity in our prefrontal cortex,” Thompson-Schill says, “so we started considering whether this might not be an unfortunate accident of nature but rather a feature of our species’ developmental path.”

The slow development of the prefrontal cortex is one reason children fail at many attention-based tasks but excel at imaginative ones. It may also aid children in rapidly acquiring new knowledge.

“There are things that are important to not filter, in particular when you are learning,” Thompson-Schill says. “If you throw out information about your environment as being irrelevant, you miss opportunities to learn about those things.”

The research was supported by the National Institutes of Health and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation.

Source: University of Pennsylvania

chat1 Comment

You are free to share this article under the Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported license.

  1. Chris

    It took you this long to figure that out?

We respect your privacy.