bias

In juvenile crime, race may affect sentence

STANFORD (US) — If people imagine a juvenile offender to be black, they are more willing to hand down harsher sentences to all juvenile offenders, a new study reports.

As the Supreme Court considers whether to further limit sentences given to juveniles, psychologists say an offender’s race shifts support for severe punishment.

“These results highlight the fragility of protections for juveniles when race is in play,” says Aneeta Rattan, postdoctoral research scholar in psychology at Stanford University and lead author of the study, which appears this week in the journal PloS One.

[sources]

Historically, the courts have protected juveniles from the most severe sentences. It has been recognized that children are different from adults—they don’t use adult reasoning and don’t have impulse control to the same degree. The Supreme Court has barred the death penalty for juveniles and, in 2010, said life without parole for non-homicide crimes violated the Constitution’s ban on cruel and unusual punishment.

Currently the court is considering two cases regarding juveniles involved in murders who were sentenced to life without parole and are weighing whether they will further limit harsh sentences for young people.

The new research was inspired, in part, by the cases most recently before the high court, says Jennifer Eberhardt, senior author of the study.

“The statistics out there indicate that there are racial disparities in sentencing juveniles who have committed severe crimes,” says Eberhardt, associate professor of psychology. “That led us to wonder, to what extent does race play a role in how people think about juvenile status?”

The study involved a nationally representative sample of 735 white Americans. Only white participants were used because whites are statistically overrepresented on juries, in the legal field and in the judiciary.

The participants were asked to read about a 14-year-old male with 17 prior juvenile convictions who brutally raped an elderly woman. Half of the respondents were told the offender was black; the other half were told he was white. The difference in race was the only change between the two stories.

Participants were asked two questions dealing with sentencing and perception.

The first: To what extent do you support life sentences without the possibility of parole for juveniles when no one was killed?

The second: How much do you believe that juveniles who commit crimes such as these should be considered less blameworthy than an adult who commits a similar crime?

Study participants who had in mind a black offender more strongly endorsed a policy of sentencing juveniles convicted of violent crimes to life in prison without parole compared to respondents who had in mind a white offender.

“The fact that imagining a particular target could influence your perceptions of a policy that would affect an entire class of people, we think, is pretty important to know,” Eberhardt says.

The black-offender group also rated juvenile offenders as more similar to adults in their culpability than did respondents in the white-offender group.

“Race is shifting how they are thinking about juveniles,” Eberhardt says. “So the protected status the offenders have as juveniles is threatened.”

The study took into account racial bias and political ideology, yet neither accounted for these effects. “The findings showed that people without racial animus or bias are affected by race as much as those with bias,” says co-author Carol Dweck, professor of psychology.

“That suggests they believe black offenders will likely be the same when they’re adults but white offenders are in a developmental period and could be very different adults. This starts breaking down the protections against the most severe sentences,” Dweck says.

The study’s authors are hopeful the findings will spur a conversation about how race affects sentencing of juveniles.

“We think about the legal world as having rules and you apply the rules equally to everyone,” says Rattan. “What we’re really showing is that there’s a potential for that to not be the case.”

“And that the rules themselves may be biased already,” Dweck adds.

More news from Stanford University: http://news.stanford.edu/

Related Articles