bat flies through trees

How bats track bugs through trees

Bats are amazingly skilled at tracking bugs through trees. How they do it could help explain how people focus on objects in cluttered scenes.

James Simmons, professor of neuroscience at Brown University, has been studying bats to figure out how their brains can target and track objects.

Experiments in his lab point to the “temporal binding hypothesis” as an explanation. The hypothesis proposes that people and animals focus on objects versus the background when a set of neurons in the brain attuned to features of an object all respond in synchrony—as if shouting in unison, “Yes, look at that!”

When the neurons do not respond together to an object, the hypothesis predicts, an object is relegated to the perceptual background.

Because bats have an especially acute need to track prey through crowded scenes, albeit with echolocation rather than vision, they have evolved to become an ideal test subject for the hypothesis.

“Sometimes the most critical questions about systems in biology that relate to humans are best approached by using an animal species whose lifestyle requires that the system in question be exaggerated in some functional sense so its qualities are more obvious,” says Simmons, who plans to discuss the research at the 2014 Cold Spring Harbor Asia Conference in Suzhou, China.

How the bat gets the bug

Here’s how he’s determined that temporal binding works in a bat.

As the bat flies it emits two spectra of sound frequencies—one high and one low—into a wide cone of space ahead of it. Within the spectra are harmonic pairs of high and low frequencies, for example 33 kilohertz and 66 kilohertz.

These harmonic pairs reflect off of objects and back to the bat’s ears, triggering a response from neurons in its brain.

Objects that reflect these harmonic pairs in perfect synchrony are the ones that stand out clearly for the bat.

But it gets more complicated. Many things could reflect the same frequency pairs back at the same time. The real question is how a target object would stand out.

The answer, Simmons writes, comes from the physics of the echolocation sound waves and how bat brains have evolved to process their signal. Those factors conspire to ensure that whatever the bat keeps front-and-center in its echolocation cone will stand out from surrounding interference.

Out-of-synch signals

The higher frequency sounds in the bat’s spectrum weaken in transit through the air more than lower frequency sounds. The bat also sends out the lower frequencies to a wider span of angles than the high frequencies.

So for any given harmonic pair, the farther away or more peripheral a reflecting object is, the weaker the higher frequency reflection in the harmonic pair will be. In the brain, Simmons writes, the bat converts this difference in signal strength into a delay in time (about 15 microseconds per decibel) so that harmonic pairs with wide differences in signal strength end up being perceived as way out of synchrony in time.

The temporal binding hypothesis predicts that the distant or peripheral objects with these out-of-synch signals will be perceived as the background while front-and-center objects that reflect back both harmonics with equal strength will rise above their desynchronized competitors.

Simmons’s research group has experimentally verified this. In key experiments (some dating back 40 years) they placed big brown bats at the base of a Y-shaped platform with a pair of objects—one a target with a food reward and the other a distractor—on the tines of the Y.

When the objects are at different distances, the bat can tell them apart and accurately crawl to the target. When the objects are equidistant, the bat becomes confused. Crucially, when the experimenters artificially weaken the high-pitched harmonic from the distracting object, even when it remains equidistant, the bat’s ability to find the target is restored.

In further experiments in 2010 and 2011, Simmons’ team showed that if they shifted the distractor object’s weakened high-frequency signal by the right amount of time (15 microseconds per decibel) they could restore the distractor’s ability to interfere with the target object by restoring the synchrony of the distractor’s harmonics.

In other words, they used the specific predictions of the hypothesis and their understanding of how it works in bats to jam the bat’s echolocation ability.

Radar and sonar

Simmons works with the US Navy on applications of bat echolocation to navigation technology. He believes the evidence he has gathered about the neuroscience of bats not only supports the temporal binding hypothesis, but also can inspire new technology.

“This is a better way to design a radar or sonar system if you need it to perform well in real-time for a small vehicle in complicated tasks,” he says.

The Office of Naval Research, the National Science Foundation, the National Institutes of Health, NASA, and the Brown Institute for Brain Science supported the research, which was published in the Journal of Experimental Biology.

Source: Brown University

chat0 Comments

You are free to share this article under the Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported license.

0 Comments

We respect your privacy.